Research Base:
The Science of Reading
Learn about the research Wilson® programs are based on.
Overview
The content of each Wilson® program is based on reading research. Scientific evidence reveals that reading is the intersection of five critical components: phonemic awareness and phonics (also referred to as alphabetics), fluency, vocabulary, and comprehension.
Recent scientific evidence reveals that teaching students to read requires instruction in all of these (Stuebing, Barth, Cirino, Francis, & Fletcher, 2008). In fact, they are specified as critical foundational reading skills in states’ college- and career-readiness standards, the National Reading Panel report (National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000), and a review of research on adult and adolescent literacy instruction by the National Institute for Literacy (Kruidenier, MacArthur, & Wrigley, 2010). Specifically, all children should receive direct, systematic instruction in these five areas during grades K-3. Together, they form the foundation upon which higher level reading skills, critical to success in college and the workforce, are built. Without the ability to fluently pull print from the page and comprehend it, students will be unable to understand complex text, one of the best predictors of college success (ACT, Inc., 2006).
Foundational Skills
While addressing the general student population, the science of reading also stipulates that these same foundational skills must be taught to students and adults with reading deficits (Kruidenier et al., 2010; Lonigan & Shanahan, 2009; NICHD, 2000). A lack of foundational skills is a major cause of poor performance in struggling readers. Word-level deficiencies limit an individual’s exposure to text, which in turn impedes the person’s vocabulary development and reading comprehension (Stanovich, 1986). To overcome these deficiencies, students and adults with reading deficits require direct, systematic instruction in the foundational skills of reading. The content of each Wilson program (Wilson Reading System®, Fundations®, and Just Words®) is based on this research.
Learn more about how Wilson’s programs incorporate the research base in these areas and view the references.
Research supports direct instruction in phonemic awareness as a critical component of an effective reading curriculum (Ehri, Nunes, Willows, et al., 2001; Lonigan et al., 2009; Melby-Lervåg et al., 2012). Wilson’s approach to phonemic awareness instruction is based on research validating best practices that maximize growth in phonemic awareness skills. Research has found that, “Instruction that taught phoneme manipulation with letters helped normal developing readers and at-risk readers acquire phonemic awareness better than phonemic awareness instruction without letters.” (National Reading Panel Report, NICHD, 2000, Chapter 2–28). Furthermore, Shaywitz found that, “While phonemic awareness refers to the sounds of spoken words, it often helps to use letters to emphasize the different sounds and to facilitate transferring this skill to reading” (2003, p 177–178). Aligned with these findings, Wilson’s programs teach letters with sounds to help students learn how to manipulate and segment sounds.
Phonemic awareness is taught using blending, segmentation, and manipulation of individual sounds, and is integrated into phonics and spelling instruction. Students use a finger tapping procedure to aid in segmenting and blending words. As students progress through the programs, instruction shifts from emphasizing phoneme segmentation to emphasizing syllable segmentation/division and applying that skill to the structure of the words being studied.
This focus on phonemic segmentation and blending is supported by research. “All of these various phonological awareness skills … the ability to separate syllables within words, the ability to segment onsets from rimes, the ability to rhyme, and the ability to segment or blend phonemes are all correlated with each other. But it is the segmenting and blending of phonemes that has functional value in reading” (Shanahan, 2015).
For more information on Wilson’s approach to phonemic awareness instruction, please see the International Dyslexia Association (IDA) Fact Sheet on Building Phoneme Awareness: Know What Matters.
Phonics instruction in the Wilson® programs is explicit and systematic. Students are directly taught the letter–sound correspondence in the written form of the English language. This aligns with recent research demonstrating that students in the primary grades make stronger gains when provided with direct phonics instruction as compared to incidental phonics instruction that is not clearly defined and explicitly taught (Stuebing et al., 2008). The same is also true of students with reading deficits (Mathes et al., 2005; Torgesen et al., 2001). Additionally, Wilson aligns with a synthesis of research studies demonstrating that interventions with the largest impact on students with reading disabilities or struggling readers were those that emphasized both phonics instruction and provided opportunities to apply phonics skills when reading connected text (Wanzek & Vaughn, 2007).
Research consistently demonstrates fluency to be a strong predictor of reading comprehension (Adams, 1990; Breznitz, 2006; Fuchs et al., 2001; Hudson et al., 2010; Kim et al., 2012; Kim et al., 2011), so Wilson’s programs have adopted fluency instruction strategies demonstrated by research to increase the text reading fluency of primary and secondary students (Chard et al., 2002; NICHD, 2000; Wexler et al., 2008). Poor readers may have difficulty marking phrase boundaries for meaning in silent reading, and therefore it may not be easy for them to determine prosody during silent reading to support comprehension of the passage (Kleiman, Winograd, & Humphrey, 1979). Also, many students lack syntactic awareness ability to phrase appropriately when reading (Benjamin & Schwanenflugel, 2010; Mokhtari & Thompson, 2006). Since prosody helps readers chunk text into syntactically appropriate units that assist them in constructing meaning (Schreiber, 1980, 1991; Schreiber & Reid, 1980), students have opportunities to work on this skill with a passage that is controlled to have words they should be able to decode. Students are directly taught a penciling technique to chunk text into meaningful phrases, and practice fluently reading connected texts with accuracy, automaticity, and prosody.
Because of the research demonstrating the difficulties struggling readers face in learning new vocabulary (Stanovich, 1986), Wilson’s programs incorporate many opportunities for students to build vocabulary and comprehension. Vocabulary is taught directly and with distributed practice. First, students are taught the meaning of select words. Selection of words is based on research on vocabulary instruction (Beck, McKeown, & Kucan, 2002). Next, students have distributed opportunities to practice reading and use vocabulary words across different contexts, which helps foster a deeper understanding of words’ meanings and students’ memory of them (Cepeda et al., 2006; Dempster, 1988).
The ultimate goal of reading is comprehension. Comprehension strategies are taught explicitly, which is particularly useful for those who struggle with comprehension (Gersten, Fuchs, Williams, & Baker, 2001). Instruction within Wilson’s programs is based on research that describes the behaviors of good and poor readers, what we know impacts one’s ability to comprehend, and research on strategies that support comprehension (Marzola, 2011). In doing so, they address both reading and listening comprehension instruction, and incorporate instruction in visualization strategies, guided close reading, and oral language instruction.
Wilson’s programs incorporate instruction in spelling skills due to research demonstrating a connection in the brain between reading and spelling activities, leading researchers to believe that spelling strongly reinforces reading (James & Engelhardt, 2012). This has led to the recommendation that as children learn to read words they also learn to spell them (Shaywitz, 2003; Carreker, 2011). For students with dyslexia, it is important that they “learn to spell words with sounds and patterns that have been previously introduced for reading and practiced” (Carreker, 2011, p.282). In each program, spelling is connected to phonics instruction and is taught through explicit instruction in spelling rules, proofreading strategies to support accurate spelling, and high-frequency sight words.
The ultimate goal of reading is comprehension. Research has found that the ability to decode words is a prerequisite for reading comprehension. Therefore, for students who need practice with decoding, whether EL or not, targeted, systematic phonics instruction is essential (Torgesen et al., 2007). Furthermore, as with all students, ELs need instruction in word learning, comprehension strategies, and academic language in order to comprehend text (Torgesen, et al., 2007). Research findings suggest that, as with native English speakers, the intervention approach for ELs should match the learner’s area of difficulty. That is, the teacher should clearly understand a student’s area(s) of difficulty or weakness (Francis, et al., 2006). For ELs who meet the criteria for placement in the Just Words® or Wilson Reading System® (WRS) program, the program content, design, and materials are developmentally appropriate for them.
Tier 3 Wilson Reading System® (WRS) students benefit from the program because it is designed specifically with the needs of diverse learners in mind. WRS aligns with guidelines from the International Dyslexia Association (2008), which recommends that students with dyslexia receive:
- explicit, direct, cumulative, intensive, and focused instruction on the structure of language;
- multisensory learning using visual, auditory, and kinesthetic-tactile pathways to enhance memory and learning of written language; and
- consistent links made between the visual, auditory, and kinesthetic-tactile pathways in learning to read and spell.
Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. MIT Press.
ACT, Inc. (2006). Reading between the lines: What the ACT reveals about college readiness in reading.
Beck, I.L., McKeown, M.G., & Kucan, L. (2002). Bringing words to life: Robust vocabulary instruction. Guilford Press.
Benjamin, R. G., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (2010). Text complexity and oral reading prosody in young readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 45(4), 388–404.
Breznitz, Z. (2006). Fluency in reading: Synchronization of processes. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Carreker, S. (2011). Teaching spelling. In J.R. Birsh (Ed.), Multisensory teaching of basic language skills (3rd ed., pp. 251–291). Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co., Inc.
Cepeda, N. J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., Wixted, J. T., & Rohrer, D. (2006). Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 354–380. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.354.
Chard, D. J., Vaughn, S., & Tyler, B.-J.. (2002). A synthesis of research on effective interventions for building reading fluency with elementary students with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35(5), 386–406. https://doi.org/10.1177/00222194020350050101
Dempster, F. N. (1988). The spacing effect: a case study in the failure to apply the results of psychological research. American Psychologist, 43, 627-634.
Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Willows, D. M., Schuster, B. V., Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z., & Shanahan, T. (2001). Phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read: Evidence from the National Reading Panel’s meta-analysis. Reading Research Quarterly, 36(3), 250–287. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.36.3.2
Francis, D. J., Rivera, M., Lesaux, N., Kieffer, M., Rivera, H. (2006). Practical Guidelines for the Education of English Language Learners: Research-Based Recommendations for Instruction and Academic Interventions. RMC Research Corporation, Center on Instruction.
Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Hosp, M. K., & Jenkins, J. R. (2001). Oral reading fluency as an indicator of reading competence: A theoretical, empirical, and historical analysis. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5(3), 239–256.
Gersten, R. F., Fuchs, L.S., Williams, J.P., & Baker, S. (2001). Teaching reading comprehension strategies to students with learning disabilities: A review of research. Review of Educational Research, 71(2), 279–320.
Hudson, R. F., Torgesen, J. K., Lane, H. B., & Turner, S. J. (2010). Relations among reading skills and sub-skills and text-level reading proficiency in developing readers. Reading and Writing, 25(2), 483–507. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9283-6
International Dyslexia Association. (2008). Just the facts: Multisensory structured language teaching. Baltimore: MD. http://www.interdys.org/ewebeditpro5/upload/MSLTeaching.pdf
James, K.H., & Engelhardt, L. (2012). The effects of handwriting experience on functional brain development in pre-literate children. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 1(1), 32–42.
Kim, Y.-S., Wagner, R. K., & Lopez, D. (2012). Developmental relations between reading fluency and reading comprehension: A longitudinal study from grade 1 to grade 2. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(1), 93-111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.03.002
Kim, Y.-S., Wagner, R. K., & Foster, E. (2011). Relations among oral reading fluency, silent reading fluency, and reading comprehension: A latent variable study of first-grade readers. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15(4), 338–362.
Kleiman, G. M., Winograd, P. N., & Humphrey, M. H. (1979). Prosody and children’s parsing of sentences (Tech. Rep. No. 123), Center for the Study of Reading, University of Illinois.
Kruidenier, J. R., MacArthur, C. A., Wrigley, H. S. (2010). Adult education literacy instruction: A review of the research. National Institute for Literacy.
Lonigan, C. J., & Shanahan, T. (2009). Executive summary of the report of the National Early Literacy Panel. In Developing early literacy: Report of the National Early Literacy Panel (pp. v–xii) . Washington DC: National Institute for Literacy.
Marzola, E. S. (2011). Strategies to improve reading comprehension in the multisensory classroom. In J.R. Birsh (Ed.), Multisensory teaching of basic language skills (3rd ed., pp. 365-403). Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co., Inc.
Mathes, P. G., Denton, C. A., Fletcher, J. M., Anthony, J. L., Francis, D. J., & Schatschneider, C. (2005). The effects of theoretically different instruction and student characteristics on the skills of struggling readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 40(2), 148–182. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.40.2.2
Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S.-A. H., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 322–352. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026744
Mokhtari, K., & Thompson, B. (2006). How problems of reading fluency and comprehension are related to difficulties in syntactic awareness skills among fifth graders. Reading Research & Instruction, 46(1), 73–96.
National Institute of Child Health & Human Development (NICHD). (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel: Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. (NIH Publication No. 004769). U. S. Government Printing Office.
Schreiber, P. A. (1980). On the acquisition of reading fluency. Journal of Reading Behavior, 12, 177–186.
Schreiber, P. A. (1991). Understanding prosody’s role in reading acquisition. Theory Into Practice, 30, 158–164.
Schreiber, P. A., & Read, C. (1980). Children’s use of phonetic cues in spelling, parsing, and—maybe—reading. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 30, 209–224.
Shaywitz, S. (2003). Overcoming dyslexia: A new and complete science-based program for reading problems at any level. Alfred A. Knopf.
Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21(4), 360–407.
Stuebing, K. K., Barth, A. E., Cirino, P. T., Francis, D. J., & Fletcher, J. M. (2008). A response to recent reanalyses of the National Reading Panel Report: Effects of systematic phonics instruction are practically significant. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(1), 123–134. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.100.1.123
Torgesen, J. K., Alexander, A. W., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Voeller, K. K. S., & Conway, T. (2001). Intensive remedial instruction for children with severe reading disabilities: Immediate and long-term outcomes from two instructional approaches. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34(1), 33–58. https://doi.org/10.1177/002221940103400104
Torgesen, J. K., Houston, D. D., Rissman, L. M., Decker, S. M., Roberts, G., Vaughn, S., Wexler, J., Francis, D. J., Rivera, M. O., Lesaux, N. (2007). Academic literacy instruction for adolescents: A guidance document from the Center on Instruction. RMC Research Corporation, Center on Instruction.
Wanzek, J., & Vaughn, S. (2007). Research-based implications from extensive early reading interventions. School Psychology Review, 36(4), 541–561.
Wexler, J., Vaughn, S., Edmonds, M, & Reutebuch, C. K. (2008). A synthesis of fluency interventions for secondary struggling readers. Reading and Writing, 21(4), 317–347. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-007-9085-7